Connect with us

Hi, what are you looking for?

NEWSMAKERS

Young children less likely than adults to see discrimination as harmful

Children see discriminatory acts — negative actions motivated by the victim’s group membership — as less serious than identical harmful acts motivated by other reasons, unrelated to the victim’s social identities. 

Photo by Kilian Seiler from Unsplash.com

Past scholarship has found discriminatory views increase as children grow older. However, a newer study shows that young children are less likely than adults to see discrimination as harmful, indicating these beliefs begin at an early age.

Moreover, children see discriminatory acts — negative actions motivated by the victim’s group membership — as less serious than identical harmful acts motivated by other reasons, unrelated to the victim’s social identities. 

The study — “In the United States, children are more likely than adults to condone discrimination” by Vivian Liu and Andrei Cimpian — appears in the journal Cognitive Psychology.

“Children frequently encounter discrimination in their daily lives—as witnesses, victims, and, sometimes, as perpetrators,” observes Liu, the paper’s lead author. “Our research reveals that, unlike adults, children… view discriminatory acts as more permissible than identical acts motivated by other reasons. This view of discrimination may lead children to engage in discriminatory behavior themselves, make them less likely to seek help when targeted by discrimination, and reduce their willingness to intervene when witnessing discrimination against others.”

However, the findings offer some reasons for optimism. They showed that by the age of 8, children begin to recognize discrimination as more harmful when it targets certain groups.

“This suggests that children can understand why discrimination is particularly harmful while also underscoring the importance of discussing these issues with children from an early age,” explains Cimpian, the senior author of the paper. 

The research consisted of four studies with nearly 600 children (aged 4 to 9) and more than 600 adults in the US. The paper’s authors presented the participants with scenarios involving harmful actions between members of different made-up groups, comparing situations where someone was harmed because of their group membership (discrimination) versus being harmed for other (personal) reasons. 

In one of the studies, for example, adult and child participants saw a member of a group (e.g. an animated circle) harming (e.g. knocking over a tower of blocks) an outgroup member (e.g. an animated square). The participants were told the harm was motivated either by dislike of the victim’s group (a discriminatory act: “… because I don’t like Squares”) or by personal dislike for the victim (“… because I don’t like you”). 

Both the perpetrator and the victim were introduced with a proper name (e.g. “Modi,” “Bosa”) so children could keep track of them throughout the story. The proper names were chosen to be uncommon in English so that children would not associate these characters with other people they knew with similar names. 

Advertisement. Scroll to continue reading.

In another study, the researchers varied whether these fictional groups were described in “essentialist” terms ( as biologically based and unchangeable)—to model how children conceive of real-world social categories such as race and gender—or instead in “temporary” terms (e.g. sports teams [“the green team”]). 

However, in one study the researchers asked participants to consider the “status” of the groups by providing information such as the following: “The Circles always get what they want and the Squares never get what they want” (i.e. “Squares” are the lower-status group) and “Sometimes the Circles get what they want and sometimes the Squares get what they want” (i.e. the groups are of equal status). 

In the majority of societies, the most common and concerning acts of discrimination occur against lower-status groups, note Cimpian and Liu. 

Younger children (roughly, before the age of 7 or 8 years) did not differentiate between discrimination against a lower- versus equal-status group. In contrast, children older than 7 or 8  began to recognize discrimination as more harmful when it targeted lower-status groups.

“Understanding how children’s views of discrimination differ from adults’ is essential for developing strategies to help them recognize and resist discriminatory behavior early in development,” Cimpian concludes. 

Advertisement
Advertisement

Like Us On Facebook

YOU MAY ALSO LIKE

NEWSMAKERS

Stress and/or anxiety stemming from everyday life seems to be the biggest culprit preventing people from having more sex (almost 32%), as well as...

Health & Wellness

The more internalized queerphobia, perceived stigma, and prejudice events that lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and queer (LGBTQ) individuals encounter means they are also more...

Travel

In California, it is now illegal to force teachers to “out” their LGBTQIA students to their parents.

Travel

In Ghana, the Supreme Court dismissed two legal challenges to a new anti-LGBTQIA legislation that would impose a three-year prison sentence for people identifying...

Advertisement